The Peter Principle: Why Things Always Go Wrong: Difference between revisions

From The Jolly Contrarian
Jump to navigation Jump to search
No edit summary
(No difference)

Revision as of 08:15, 10 July 2021

The Jolly Contrarian’s book review service™


Index: Click to expand:

Comments? Questions? Suggestions? Requests? Insults? We’d love to 📧 hear from you.
Sign up for our newsletter.

The Peter Principle, by Dr. Laurence J. Peter and Raymond Hull

This classic satire of modern management is, of course, largely correct and — but for some rather dated scenarios and value judgments — remains highly relevant to explain the mess of modern middle management.

Re can re-dress two of its central concepts in terms a millennial might understand: the hierarchy, and the Peter Principle itself, that in a hierarchy, everyone tends to rises to one’s own level of incompetence.[1]

Hierarchy

A hierarchy is a social collective that, at some point, spontaneously gathered and self-organised to pursue a common purpose, and in doing so created its own autonomous reason for being, quite independent of the satisfaction of the purpose. A contemporary snowflake might recognise it as a power structure. That spontaneity is a kind of necessary condition for the formation of a hierarchy, but once established, it quickly withers, and the self-organisation of the hierarchy — at the hands of those at the top of it — takes over. The predominating mission of the hierarchy becomes not the pursuit of the original purpose — such as it is, it may well change quite markedly over time — but the defence of the hierarchy itself. Thus, the first commandment of hierarchical life: the hierarchy must be preserved.

Rising to incompetence as a form of natural selection

Consider the underlying formulation of evolution by natural selection:

if useful variations occur when an organism breeds, individuals with those variations will have a better chance of survival than those without and their own offspring will also be more likely to have those same useful variations

Of course the complication is that genetic features which are useful at this point in evolutionary history will not always be so useful at at that one.

Now if we substitute “breeding” for “promotion”, we see that, in a population of workers in a hierarchy, those with traits that are useful for promotion — note, it is for “promotion”, not “once promoted” — will tend to fare better than those that do not. Traits “useful for promotion” can only be judged from traits observable at one’s current rank — basic competencies, in other words — so those most competent at their current role will be the ones suitable for promotion. If they turn out to be competent at their promoted role, too, they will remain in the game for onward promotion, they remain in the upward flow; if they don’t —if they are bad at it — they will get stuck. Hence the “Peter Principle”, which on this read is as self-evidently, mathematically true[2] as is evolution by natural selection.


See also

  1. I have wokified this a little bit from its 1969 formulation; the key change is that it is not just employees, but anyone in a hierarchy. This is a consequence of the agency problem.
  2. mathematically, not scientifically. Scientific truths aren’t allowed to be self-evident. See falsifiability.